![]() Umschlagbild: Die hochbeinigen und schnelläufigen Wüstenameisen der Gattung Cataglyphis gehören zu den auffälligsten Erscheinungen der Insektenfauna altweltlicher Trockengebiete. Auf ihren Jagdläufen legen sie keine Duftspuren, sondern steuern ihren Kurs nach dem Prinzip der Vektornavigation mit Hilfe eines astronomischen Kompasses. Das Bild zeigt eine Cataglyphis bicolor beim Verlassen ihrer Nestöffnung. (Aufnahme: R. Wehner) |
Inhaltsverzeichnis
|
1. Skylight patterns, receptor arrays and neural images.
a) Skylight patterns result from the scattering of unpolarized
light of the sun within the earth's atmosphere. These patterns include
variations of intensity (I), degree of polarization (D), and angle of polarization
(c) as well as how all these variables depend
on the wavelength of light (l (Figs. 12-15,
Plate 1).
b) The celestial system of coordinates is mapped on to
the compound eyes of Cataglyphis (Fig. 23). This is accomplished by (1)
exploiting the optical phenomenon of the luminous pseudopupil (Plate 2)
in order to determine the directions of view of individual ommatidia, and
(2) measuring the angle of pitch and roll by which the ant's head is adjusted
relative to the transverse and longitudinal body axis, respectively (Figs.
19 and 53). As during navigation both angles are kept rather constant,
it is only when the ant changes its compass bearing, i.e. rotates about
its dorsoventral body axis, that the celestial coordinates move across
the eye. In addition, by referring to the directions of view of the marginal
ommatidia of both eyes, the range of binocular overlap as well as the area
covered by neither eye are calculated (Fig. 21; for maps exhibiting the
density of photoreceptors see Fig. 24).
c) The anatomical organization of the retina is described
for both the worker honeybee (Figs. 29, 30, 31 a~) and the ant Cataglyphis
(Figs. 31 d-f, 33, 34, 36, 3941). Special emphasis is placed on structural
differences as they occur among the retinulae of different parts of the
eye (Figs. 31 and 37). The specialized ommatidia present in the dorsal
rim area of the eye are characterized by (1) relatively large rhabdoms,
(2) an orthogonal arrangement of microvillar directions within the rhabdom,
and (3) a fan-like arrangement of the transverse axes of the rhabdoms within
the retina (Fig. 32). (4) The dorsal rim area looks contralaterally (Fig.
35). In addition, the retinulae of the dorsal rim area of Apis (5) consist
of nine long photoreceptor cells that are (6) not twisted (Fig. 29) and
(7) characterized by large visual fields (Fig. 28). The latter property
is due to structural specializations within the overlying corneal lenses
(Fig. 27). In the remainder of the bee's eye the retinulae are twisted
(Fig. 29) and comprised of one short and eight long photoreceptor cells.
d) The spectral sensitivity functions of the ultraviolet
and green receptors are determined for both ants and bees (Fig. 25). Untwisted
ultraviolet receptors exhibit high polarizational sensitivity (5 < PS
< 10), whereas the twisted long ultraviolet receptors of the bee's retina
are insensitive to polarized light (PS < 2).
e) In both bees and ants green and ultraviolet receptors
give rise to short and long visual fibres terminating in the lamina and
the medulla, respectively. Different structural types of second order interneurons
(monopolar cells) are described (Fig. 41).
f) Neural response patterns (neural images) are designed
which result when an array of polarizationally sensitive opponent cells
(P- [polaroticity-] interneurons) views the pattern of polarized light
in the sky (Fig. 42, Plates 3 and 4).
2. Properties of the celestial compass.
a) In the visual systems of bees and ants the detection
of polarized skylight is exclusively mediated by the ultraviolet receptors
(Figs. 44 and 47). In contrast, the unpolarized light of the sun can be
used as a compass cue when only the green receptors are stimulated (Figs.
4547). As light from the radiant sky is rich in ultraviolet but light reflected
from the surface of the earth is not (Plate 5), it seems likely that the
ultraviolet receptors have evolved in the functional context of skylight
navigation. It might have been only later (e.g. in coevolutionary interaction
with the development of ultraviolet floral patterns of angiosperm plants)
that they became incorporated into a colour vision system used in the detection
of terrestrial objects.
b) What different parts of the eye contribute to skylight
navigation is studied by occluding specific parts of the eye with paint
(Figs. 48 and 54) or screening off certain parts of the sky (Figs. 9, 11
and 53). The results are summarized in Figs. 49 and 50 for Cataglyphis
and Figs. 55 and 56 for Apis. The dorsal rim area (see Sect. lc) is of
special importance for the detection of polarized skylight.
c) Ants are informed rather well about the daily movement
of the sun's azimuth, i.e. the symmetry line of the skylight patterns (Fig.
57). However, they slightly underestimate the highest rates of movement
of the sun's azimuth at noon and overestimate lower ones (Fig. 58). This
is consistent with the hypothesis that some kind of interpolation procedure
contributes to the ant's internal representation of the movement of the
sun. When tested at night, the ants interpret the moon as the sun (Fig.
59).
d) How does the insect derive compass information
from skylight patterns? (1) The insect could use some knowledge about
the physics of Rayleigh scattering and deduce the position of the sun as
the intersection point of at least two great circles running at right angles
to the e-vector directions measured in at least two points in the sky.
(2) The insect could be informed about the distribution of e-vector directions
across the sky, i.e. rely on an e-vector map. In both cases the insect
should exhibit a certain type of ambiguous (bimodal) orientation when confronted
with a single point in the sky (Fig. 60). This never occurs in either ants
or bees. When a horizontal e-vector is presented, the insect orients correctly
by interpreting the horizontal e-vector as being positioned along the antisolar
meridian. However, when e-vectors are offered that are oriented in other
than horizontal directions, small but consistent errors occur. Apparently,
insects rely on some generalized version of the e-vector pattern in the
sky. If this holds true as a general strategy (even when the insect is
able to view large parts of the sky), the next question can be phrased
as follows: How are the insect's peripheral neural images such as the ones
shown in Plates 3 and 4 transformed to more central neural images which
the navigating insect must compare continuously with its internal (generalized)
master image of the sky?
In addition to polarizational cues, insects refer to
spectral cues in the sky.
e) The possible mechanisms of celestial and terrestrial
navigation (Figs. 61-63) are compared briefly. In both cases insects do
not seem to rely on three-dimensional representations of their visual surround
(i.e. the celestial hemisphere or the arrangement of terrestrial objects
around the insect), but on the analysis of two-dimensional images and sequences
of such images.